Social Engineering Blogs

An Aggregator for Blogs About Social Engineering and Related Fields

The Humintell Blog October 25, 2018

Basic Emotions of Violence

As may be unsurprising to many of you, reading basic emotions in others may be a key to predicting violence.

Humintell has written for years on the importance of basic emotions, but does understanding these emotions help better understand or predict violent behavior? Drs. David Matsumoto, Hyi Sung Hwang, and Mark Frank all agree with a resounding “yes” in a blog article written for the Federal Bureau of Investigation. Their observations ring true as an important explication of how fundamental our basic emotions can be.

Different emotions can mean categorically different things. For instance, contempt, disgust, and anger all refer to feelings of emotion towards an out-group, but they mean different things. Anger is usually channeled at somebody’s actions, while disgust and contempt are focused on the other person themselves.

They proceed to elaborate on the role that disgust can have in predicting violence. Disgust, they write, results in the desire to eliminate the disgusting object. Certainly, this can have horrifying consequences when applied to other people, and they point to genocidal leaders, terrorists, and mass shooters as consistently evincing disgust in public speeches or videos.

Contempt is a similar phenomenon as disgust, in that it focuses on another’s actions as they relate to status and hierarchy. That person is often seen as not fitting the status that they claim. This is a different sort of emotion, but it also leads to a disposition against the individual not just their actions.

Anger is often seen as an important predictor of violence, but given the context of terrorism and genocide, disgust may be the particularly salient emotion. That said, anger can easily be turned into disgust. Since anger is focused on a given situation or action, its conversion to disgust can involve a larger shift in attitude from a specific instance to a more general disposition towards an out-group.

Still, while disgust and contempt are being emphasized, anger has a crucial role here too. It is through anger that the hesitation against action is overcome. While an individual may be motivated to violence through disgust or contempt, it is through anger that they actually make this decision to act rather than to refrain.

This is all the case at a very physiological level as well. We act out of anger because anger increases our heart rate and blood flow. Similarly, disgust is a deep-seated emotion born out of our concerns for parasites and the need to ensure the safety of food and water.

It may be helpful to see these relationships more simply. Contempt and disgust lead to the breakdown of relations between groups or individuals, and anger leads to those same individuals actually acting on such hostility. This underscores the need to be able to not just detect anger but to read people’s emotions more broadly.

Filed Under: Emotion

The Humintell Blog October 19, 2018

Threat in the Golden Years

As we age, we tend to look more fondly on the world and on our memories, but does that mean we can’t detect threat?

While a great deal of research indicates that older individuals tend to focus on pleasurable or non-threatening aspects of experience and memories, it is unclear whether this means they are less able to recognize threating behaviors in other individuals. This is what Drs. Mara Mather and Marisa Knight sought to determine in a 2006 article.

This question tries to get at even deeper questions as to how the brain processes threatening information. Because our threat detection is mediated by the brain’s amygdala, perhaps this functions less effectively as we age. Alternatively, this may be due to “strategic processing” where older individuals’ brains use positive inclinations to better process information.

In order to critically evaluate this question, both young and older participants were recruited for an experimental study, asking preliminary questions to confirm that the older participants did tend to experience a more positive affect. Each participant was then exposed to a selection of nine facial images.

Half of the participants were only shown neutral faces, while another half had one emotional expression mixed into these neutral faces. Some of these treatment faces were threatening, but others were sad or friendly. After being exposed to the treatment, each individual was asked to identify whether the face appeared to be threatening.

Contrary to some of the theoretical expectations, age seemed to make no impact on accurate identification. They were also able to recognize threatening faces more quickly, confirming previous research, but age did not seem to make a difference here.

So, what does this tell us about our ability to detect threat? And more specifically, how does this help us do so?

First, it tells us that threat detection is a very fundamental underlying process in our brains. While a great deal of cognitive processes change as we age, it is notable that this one does not seem to.

Second, the almost instantaneous nature of threat detection not only underscores its fundamental role but also gives us practical tips on how to detect threat. Just like facial recognition, our brain automatically processes faces and gives us certain intuitions.

However, while it is good to trust these intuitions, they might not always be accurate. Our brains are pretty incredible, but they are not infallible. This is a great reason to get real, professional training to teach our brain what to look for. This can make an already incredible skill even more formidable!

Filed Under: Emotion, Science

The Humintell Blog October 11, 2018

Can You Spot the Anger?

While it may be tricky to say if a face is threatening, our brain may already be deciding for us.

This may sound far-fetched, but as Dr. Harald Schupp and a team of researchers found in 2004, we are hardwired on an evolutionary level to experience a fear response upon detecting perceived threat in another face. While we may not know what is happening, at a physiological level our body certainly reacts.

This research is deeply rooted in our evolutionary history. As we have previously written, many of our universal emotional expressions are based in how our faces evolved, such as narrowed eyes to tune out disgusting imagery. Dr. Schupp applies this sort of insight to how we react to a threatening face.

Essentially, the perception of threat in another’s face is deeply intertwined with our fear response. Past research found that we detect threat in faces much more quickly than more positive emotions, and our brains quickly prepare for the worst. Dr. Schupp’s work drives this insight further by looking at the basic neurological response that is triggered upon perceiving threat in another’s face.

In the study, a small group of participants were recruited and subsequently exposed to a series of images representing basic emotions: threat/anger, happiness, and a neutral face. The participants were asked to rate each face based on the extent to which it seemed either friendly or threatening, but they were only exposed to the face for a brief moment.

During this process, brain activity was monitored in order to observe changes in activity and intensity at the neurological level. This allowed them to track both the differences between brain activity in threatening and friendly recognitions and also the speed at which both occur.

Consistent with their hypotheses, the study found that people’s brains show markedly different activity almost immediately. However, the difference between a threat recognition and a friendly recognition was much greater than that between friendly and neutral, suggesting that our brains are responding in a categorically way to threat than to other facial expressions.

Similarly, while threatening faces were noticed much more quickly, they were also marked by prolonged analysis shown by activity in the brain. Our recognition does not stop at recognizing threat, as it does when we recognize a friendly visage. Instead, it continues to process the stimulus in order to formulate an accurate response, such as flight or fight.

You may be wondering how such a dense neurological analysis fits into practical tips for detecting aggression in others or how any of this relates to the goal of being more aware of domestic violence.

In fact, the understanding that our brain has a deep and instinctive reaction to threat helps us be better aware of what is going on instinctively when we see a face. Some faces may inspire a sense of subconscious anxiety or consternation, and this may very well be linked to our neurological recognition processes.

Just as we mentioned last week, better understanding how we recognize threat is incredibly important both for those who are at risk for violence but also for observers and friends who may notice violent potential in others. This is just one path towards a better understanding these issues, with another being formal training in threat detection with Humintell.

Filed Under: Emotion

  • « Previous Page
  • 1
  • …
  • 35
  • 36
  • 37
  • 38
  • 39
  • …
  • 68
  • Next Page »

About

Welcome to an aggregator for blogs about social engineering and related fields. Feel free to take a look around, and make sure to visit the original sites.

If you would like to suggest a site or contact us, use the links below.

Contact

  • Contact
  • Suggest a Site
  • Remove a Site

© Copyright 2025 Social Engineering Blogs · All Rights Reserved ·